-
Notifications
You must be signed in to change notification settings - Fork 3
/
bib-refs.bib
4045 lines (3752 loc) · 236 KB
/
bib-refs.bib
1
2
3
4
5
6
7
8
9
10
11
12
13
14
15
16
17
18
19
20
21
22
23
24
25
26
27
28
29
30
31
32
33
34
35
36
37
38
39
40
41
42
43
44
45
46
47
48
49
50
51
52
53
54
55
56
57
58
59
60
61
62
63
64
65
66
67
68
69
70
71
72
73
74
75
76
77
78
79
80
81
82
83
84
85
86
87
88
89
90
91
92
93
94
95
96
97
98
99
100
101
102
103
104
105
106
107
108
109
110
111
112
113
114
115
116
117
118
119
120
121
122
123
124
125
126
127
128
129
130
131
132
133
134
135
136
137
138
139
140
141
142
143
144
145
146
147
148
149
150
151
152
153
154
155
156
157
158
159
160
161
162
163
164
165
166
167
168
169
170
171
172
173
174
175
176
177
178
179
180
181
182
183
184
185
186
187
188
189
190
191
192
193
194
195
196
197
198
199
200
201
202
203
204
205
206
207
208
209
210
211
212
213
214
215
216
217
218
219
220
221
222
223
224
225
226
227
228
229
230
231
232
233
234
235
236
237
238
239
240
241
242
243
244
245
246
247
248
249
250
251
252
253
254
255
256
257
258
259
260
261
262
263
264
265
266
267
268
269
270
271
272
273
274
275
276
277
278
279
280
281
282
283
284
285
286
287
288
289
290
291
292
293
294
295
296
297
298
299
300
301
302
303
304
305
306
307
308
309
310
311
312
313
314
315
316
317
318
319
320
321
322
323
324
325
326
327
328
329
330
331
332
333
334
335
336
337
338
339
340
341
342
343
344
345
346
347
348
349
350
351
352
353
354
355
356
357
358
359
360
361
362
363
364
365
366
367
368
369
370
371
372
373
374
375
376
377
378
379
380
381
382
383
384
385
386
387
388
389
390
391
392
393
394
395
396
397
398
399
400
401
402
403
404
405
406
407
408
409
410
411
412
413
414
415
416
417
418
419
420
421
422
423
424
425
426
427
428
429
430
431
432
433
434
435
436
437
438
439
440
441
442
443
444
445
446
447
448
449
450
451
452
453
454
455
456
457
458
459
460
461
462
463
464
465
466
467
468
469
470
471
472
473
474
475
476
477
478
479
480
481
482
483
484
485
486
487
488
489
490
491
492
493
494
495
496
497
498
499
500
501
502
503
504
505
506
507
508
509
510
511
512
513
514
515
516
517
518
519
520
521
522
523
524
525
526
527
528
529
530
531
532
533
534
535
536
537
538
539
540
541
542
543
544
545
546
547
548
549
550
551
552
553
554
555
556
557
558
559
560
561
562
563
564
565
566
567
568
569
570
571
572
573
574
575
576
577
578
579
580
581
582
583
584
585
586
587
588
589
590
591
592
593
594
595
596
597
598
599
600
601
602
603
604
605
606
607
608
609
610
611
612
613
614
615
616
617
618
619
620
621
622
623
624
625
626
627
628
629
630
631
632
633
634
635
636
637
638
639
640
641
642
643
644
645
646
647
648
649
650
651
652
653
654
655
656
657
658
659
660
661
662
663
664
665
666
667
668
669
670
671
672
673
674
675
676
677
678
679
680
681
682
683
684
685
686
687
688
689
690
691
692
693
694
695
696
697
698
699
700
701
702
703
704
705
706
707
708
709
710
711
712
713
714
715
716
717
718
719
720
721
722
723
724
725
726
727
728
729
730
731
732
733
734
735
736
737
738
739
740
741
742
743
744
745
746
747
748
749
750
751
752
753
754
755
756
757
758
759
760
761
762
763
764
765
766
767
768
769
770
771
772
773
774
775
776
777
778
779
780
781
782
783
784
785
786
787
788
789
790
791
792
793
794
795
796
797
798
799
800
801
802
803
804
805
806
807
808
809
810
811
812
813
814
815
816
817
818
819
820
821
822
823
824
825
826
827
828
829
830
831
832
833
834
835
836
837
838
839
840
841
842
843
844
845
846
847
848
849
850
851
852
853
854
855
856
857
858
859
860
861
862
863
864
865
866
867
868
869
870
871
872
873
874
875
876
877
878
879
880
881
882
883
884
885
886
887
888
889
890
891
892
893
894
895
896
897
898
899
900
901
902
903
904
905
906
907
908
909
910
911
912
913
914
915
916
917
918
919
920
921
922
923
924
925
926
927
928
929
930
931
932
933
934
935
936
937
938
939
940
941
942
943
944
945
946
947
948
949
950
951
952
953
954
955
956
957
958
959
960
961
962
963
964
965
966
967
968
969
970
971
972
973
974
975
976
977
978
979
980
981
982
983
984
985
986
987
988
989
990
991
992
993
994
995
996
997
998
999
1000
@article{dinges_assessing_1985,
title = {Assessing performance upon abrupt awakening from naps during quasi-continuous operations},
volume = {17},
number = {1},
urldate = {2015-07-16},
journal = {Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, \& Computers},
author = {Dinges, David F. and Orne, Martin T. and Orne, Emily Carota},
year = {1985},
pages = {37--45},
file = {Dinges et al_1985_Assessing performance upon abrupt awakening from naps during quasi-continuous.pdf:C\:\\Users\\Raphael\\AppData\\Roaming\\Mozilla\\Firefox\\Profiles\\mm1r2zj7.default-1442859820820\\zotero\\storage\\SGMKNXME\\Dinges et al_1985_Assessing performance upon abrupt awakening from naps during quasi-continuous.pdf:application/pdf}
}
@article{silber_visual_2007,
title = {The visual scoring of sleep in adults},
volume = {3},
issn = {1550-9389},
abstract = {The 1968 Rechtschaffen and Kales (R \& K) sleep scoring manual was published 15 years after REM sleep was discovered. Advances in the ensuing 28 years warranted a re-look at visual scoring of sleep stages. This paper describes the work of the AASM Visual Scoring Task Force, including methodology, a literature review and the rationale behind the new rules. Reliability studies of R \& K scoring were reviewed; reliability was low for stage one and moderate for slow wave sleep. Evidence indicated that K complexes and slow waves are expressed maximal frontally, spindles centrally and alpha rhythm over the occipital region. Three derivations of EEG, two of electro-oculography, and one of chin EMG were recommended. Scoring by 30-second epochs was retained. New terminology for sleep stages was proposed. Attenuation of alpha rhythm was determined to be the most valid electrophysiological marker of sleep onset. Alternative measures were proposed for non-alpha generating subjects. K complexes associated with arousals were determined to be insufficient alone to define the new stage N2. No evidence was found to justify dividing slow wave sleep into two stages. No reasons were found to alter the current slow wave amplitude criteria at any age. The phenomena of REM sleep were defined. The rules for defining onset and termination of REM sleep periods were simplified. Movement time was eliminated and major body movements defined. Studies are needed to test the reliability of the new rules. Future advances in technology may require modification of these rules with time.},
language = {eng},
number = {2},
journal = {Journal of clinical sleep medicine: JCSM: official publication of the American Academy of Sleep Medicine},
author = {Silber, Michael H. and Ancoli-Israel, Sonia and Bonnet, Michael H. and Chokroverty, Sudhansu and Grigg-Damberger, Madeleine M. and Hirshkowitz, Max and Kapen, Sheldon and Keenan, Sharon A. and Kryger, Meir H. and Penzel, Thomas and Pressman, Mark R. and Iber, Conrad},
month = mar,
year = {2007},
pmid = {17557422},
keywords = {Adult, Electroencephalography, Electrooculography, Humans, Polysomnography, Research, Research Design, Sleep, Sleep Disorders, Sleep, REM, Sleep Stages},
pages = {121--131},
file = {Silber et al_2007_The visual scoring of sleep in adults.pdf:C\:\\Users\\Raphael\\AppData\\Roaming\\Mozilla\\Firefox\\Profiles\\mm1r2zj7.default-1442859820820\\zotero\\storage\\TPR9J55M\\Silber et al_2007_The visual scoring of sleep in adults.pdf:application/pdf}
}
@article{blagrove_assessing_2011,
title = {Assessing the {Dream}-{Lag} {Effect} for {REM} and {NREM} {Stage} 2 {Dreams}},
volume = {6},
issn = {1932-6203},
url = {http://dx.plos.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0026708},
doi = {10.1371/journal.pone.0026708},
language = {en},
number = {10},
urldate = {2015-05-06},
journal = {PLoS ONE},
author = {Blagrove, Mark and Fouquet, Nathalie C. and Henley-Einion, Josephine A. and Pace-Schott, Edward F. and Davies, Anna C. and Neuschaffer, Jennifer L. and Turnbull, Oliver H.},
editor = {Tsakiris, Manos},
month = oct,
year = {2011},
pages = {e26708},
file = {Blagrove et al_2011_Assessing the Dream-Lag Effect for REM and NREM Stage 2 Dreams.pdf:C\:\\Users\\Raphael\\AppData\\Roaming\\Mozilla\\Firefox\\Profiles\\mm1r2zj7.default-1442859820820\\zotero\\storage\\B85XZXA9\\Blagrove et al_2011_Assessing the Dream-Lag Effect for REM and NREM Stage 2 Dreams.pdf:application/pdf}
}
@article{tassi_sleep_2000,
title = {Sleep inertia},
volume = {4},
issn = {10870792},
url = {http://linkinghub.elsevier.com/retrieve/pii/S1087079200900984},
doi = {10.1053/smrv.2000.0098},
language = {en},
number = {4},
urldate = {2015-05-06},
journal = {Sleep Medicine Reviews},
author = {Tassi, P and Muzet, A},
month = aug,
year = {2000},
pages = {341--353},
file = {Tassi_Muzet_2000_Sleep inertia.pdf:C\:\\Users\\Raphael\\AppData\\Roaming\\Mozilla\\Firefox\\Profiles\\mm1r2zj7.default-1442859820820\\zotero\\storage\\D32633Q5\\Tassi_Muzet_2000_Sleep inertia.pdf:application/pdf}
}
@article{wamsley_dreaming_2010,
title = {Dreaming and offline memory processing},
volume = {20},
issn = {0960-9822},
url = {http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0960982210013606},
doi = {10.1016/j.cub.2010.10.045},
abstract = {Summary
The activities of the mind and brain never cease. Although many of our waking hours are spent processing sensory input and executing behavioral responses, moments of unoccupied rest free us to wander through thoughts of the past and future, create daydreams, and imagine fictitious scenarios. During sleep, when attention to sensory input is at a minimum, the mind continues to process information, using memory fragments to create the images, thoughts, and narratives that we commonly call ‘dreaming’. Far from being a random or meaningless distraction, spontaneous cognition during states of sleep and resting wakefulness appears to serve important functions related to processing past memories and planning for the future. From single-cell recordings in rodents to behavioral studies in humans, recent studies in the neurosciences suggest a new conception of dreaming as part of a continuum of adaptive cognitive processing occurring across the full range of mind/brain states.},
number = {23},
urldate = {2015-05-06},
journal = {Current Biology},
author = {Wamsley, Erin J. and Stickgold, Robert},
year = {2010},
keywords = {Memory consolidation},
pages = {R1010--R1013},
file = {ScienceDirect Snapshot:C\:\\Users\\Raphael\\AppData\\Roaming\\Mozilla\\Firefox\\Profiles\\mm1r2zj7.default-1442859820820\\zotero\\storage\\8N2XB87C\\S0960982210013606.html:text/html;Wamsley_Stickgold_2010_Dreaming and offline memory processing.pdf:C\:\\Users\\Raphael\\AppData\\Roaming\\Mozilla\\Firefox\\Profiles\\mm1r2zj7.default-1442859820820\\zotero\\storage\\4NQ2VBMU\\Wamsley_Stickgold_2010_Dreaming and offline memory processing.pdf:application/pdf}
}
@article{dijk_dreaming_2015,
title = {Dreaming, memory, insomnia and the exploding attention for sleep},
volume = {24},
issn = {09621105},
url = {http://doi.wiley.com/10.1111/jsr.12331},
doi = {10.1111/jsr.12331},
language = {en},
number = {4},
urldate = {2015-09-11},
journal = {Journal of Sleep Research},
author = {Dijk, Derk-Jan},
month = aug,
year = {2015},
pages = {359--359},
file = {Dijk - 2015 - Dreaming, memory, insomnia and the exploding atten.pdf:C\:\\Users\\Raphael\\AppData\\Roaming\\Mozilla\\Firefox\\Profiles\\mm1r2zj7.default-1442859820820\\zotero\\storage\\DBV3S7UF\\Dijk - 2015 - Dreaming, memory, insomnia and the exploding atten.pdf:application/pdf;Dijk_2015_Dreaming, memory, insomnia and the exploding attention for sleep.pdf:C\:\\Users\\Raphael\\AppData\\Roaming\\Mozilla\\Firefox\\Profiles\\mm1r2zj7.default-1442859820820\\zotero\\storage\\C5SG7EMN\\Dijk_2015_Dreaming, memory, insomnia and the exploding attention for sleep.pdf:application/pdf}
}
@article{van_dijk_intrinsic_2010,
title = {Intrinsic functional connectivity as a tool for human connectomics: theory, properties, and optimization},
volume = {103},
issn = {1522-1598},
shorttitle = {Intrinsic functional connectivity as a tool for human connectomics},
doi = {10.1152/jn.00783.2009},
abstract = {Resting state functional connectivity MRI (fcMRI) is widely used to investigate brain networks that exhibit correlated fluctuations. While fcMRI does not provide direct measurement of anatomic connectivity, accumulating evidence suggests it is sufficiently constrained by anatomy to allow the architecture of distinct brain systems to be characterized. fcMRI is particularly useful for characterizing large-scale systems that span distributed areas (e.g., polysynaptic cortical pathways, cerebro-cerebellar circuits, cortical-thalamic circuits) and has complementary strengths when contrasted with the other major tool available for human connectomics-high angular resolution diffusion imaging (HARDI). We review what is known about fcMRI and then explore fcMRI data reliability, effects of preprocessing, analysis procedures, and effects of different acquisition parameters across six studies (n = 98) to provide recommendations for optimization. Run length (2-12 min), run structure (1 12-min run or 2 6-min runs), temporal resolution (2.5 or 5.0 s), spatial resolution (2 or 3 mm), and the task (fixation, eyes closed rest, eyes open rest, continuous word-classification) were varied. Results revealed moderate to high test-retest reliability. Run structure, temporal resolution, and spatial resolution minimally influenced fcMRI results while fixation and eyes open rest yielded stronger correlations as contrasted to other task conditions. Commonly used preprocessing steps involving regression of nuisance signals minimized nonspecific (noise) correlations including those associated with respiration. The most surprising finding was that estimates of correlation strengths stabilized with acquisition times as brief as 5 min. The brevity and robustness of fcMRI positions it as a powerful tool for large-scale explorations of genetic influences on brain architecture. We conclude by discussing the strengths and limitations of fcMRI and how it can be combined with HARDI techniques to support the emerging field of human connectomics.},
language = {eng},
number = {1},
journal = {Journal of Neurophysiology},
author = {Van Dijk, Koene R. A. and Hedden, Trey and Venkataraman, Archana and Evans, Karleyton C. and Lazar, Sara W. and Buckner, Randy L.},
month = jan,
year = {2010},
pmid = {19889849},
pmcid = {PMC2807224},
keywords = {Adolescent, Adult, Attention, Brain, Databases as Topic, Fixation, Ocular, Humans, Magnetic Resonance Imaging, Models, Neurological, Motor Activity, Neural Pathways, Oxygen, Respiration, Rest, Signal Processing, Computer-Assisted, Time Factors, Visual Perception, Young Adult},
pages = {297--321},
annote = {Annotations extraites (30/07/2016 à 18:34:48)"We review what is known about fcMRI and then explore fcMRI data reliability, effects of preprocessing, analysis procedures, and effects of different acquisition parameters across six studies (n 98) to provide recommendations for optimization" (Van Dijk et al 2010:297)"Run length (2-12 min" (Van Dijk et al 2010:297)"2.5 or 5.0 s" (Van Dijk et al 2010:297)"fixation, eyes closed rest, eyes open rest, continuous word-classification" (Van Dijk et al 2010:297)"Results revealed moderate to high test-retest reliability" (Van Dijk et al 2010:297)"The most surprising finding was that estimates of correlation strengths stabilized with acquisition times as brief as 5 min" (Van Dijk et al 2010:297)"Functional connectivity is based on the observation that distant brain regions often show strong correlations in their activity levels" (Van Dijk et al 2010:298)"functional correlations between widely distributed brain regions are consistently observed in analyses of fMRI time series data (see Fox and Raichle 2007 for review)." (Van Dijk et al 2010:298)},
file = {Van Dijk et al_2010_Intrinsic functional connectivity as a tool for human connectomics.pdf:C\:\\Users\\Raphael\\AppData\\Roaming\\Mozilla\\Firefox\\Profiles\\mm1r2zj7.default-1442859820820\\zotero\\storage\\BPU6N2FE\\Van Dijk et al_2010_Intrinsic functional connectivity as a tool for human connectomics.pdf:application/pdf}
}
@article{lajnef_learning_2015,
title = {Learning machines and sleeping brains: {Automatic} sleep stage classification using decision-tree multi-class support vector machines},
volume = {250},
issn = {01650270},
shorttitle = {Learning machines and sleeping brains},
url = {http://linkinghub.elsevier.com/retrieve/pii/S0165027015000230},
doi = {10.1016/j.jneumeth.2015.01.022},
language = {en},
urldate = {2015-08-07},
journal = {Journal of Neuroscience Methods},
author = {Lajnef, Tarek and Chaibi, Sahbi and Ruby, Perrine and Aguera, Pierre-Emmanuel and Eichenlaub, Jean-Baptiste and Samet, Mounir and Kachouri, Abdennaceur and Jerbi, Karim},
month = jul,
year = {2015},
pages = {94--105},
file = {Lajnef et al_2015_Learning machines and sleeping brains.pdf:C\:\\Users\\Raphael\\AppData\\Roaming\\Mozilla\\Firefox\\Profiles\\mm1r2zj7.default-1442859820820\\zotero\\storage\\ZUHVAB48\\Lajnef et al_2015_Learning machines and sleeping brains.pdf:application/pdf}
}
@article{american_sleep_disorders_association_eeg_1992,
title = {{EEG} arousals: scoring rules and examples},
volume = {15},
shorttitle = {{EEG} arousals},
journal = {Sleep},
author = {{American Sleep Disorders Association}},
year = {1992},
pages = {74--101},
file = {American Sleep Disorders Association_1992_EEG arousals.pdf:C\:\\Users\\Raphael\\AppData\\Roaming\\Mozilla\\Firefox\\Profiles\\mm1r2zj7.default-1442859820820\\zotero\\storage\\VB5ZD54I\\American Sleep Disorders Association_1992_EEG arousals.pdf:application/pdf}
}
@article{tassi_effects_1992,
title = {Effects of noise on sleep inertia as a function of circadian placement of a one-hour nap},
volume = {75},
issn = {0031-5125},
doi = {10.2466/pms.1992.75.1.291},
abstract = {The purpose of the present study was to analyse the arousing effects of noise on sleep inertia as a function of circadian placement of a one-hour nap. In a first experiment, we measured the effects of sleep inertia in a neutral acoustic environment after a one-hour nap placed either at 0100 or 0400 on response time during a spatial memory test. In a second experiment were analysed the effects of an intense continuous noise on sleep inertia. The results showed that noise produced a total abolition of sleep inertia after an early nap (0000 to 0100). This may be due to the arousing effect of noise; however, results are less clear after a late nap 0300 to 0400 as noise seems to be ineffective. This result is discussed in terms of either a function of time-of-day effect or of prior sleep intensity. Moreover, our data suggest a possible interaction of noise with partial sleep deprivation leading to a slight deleterious effect those subjects who did not sleep at all.},
language = {eng},
number = {1},
journal = {Perceptual and Motor Skills},
author = {Tassi, P. and Nicolas, A. and Dewasmes, G. and Eschenlauer, R. and Ehrhart, J. and Salame, P. and Muzet, A. and Libert, J. P.},
month = aug,
year = {1992},
pmid = {1528684},
keywords = {Adult, Arousal, Attention, Auditory Perception, Circadian Rhythm, Humans, Male, Problem Solving, Psychomotor Performance, Reaction Time, Sleep Stages},
pages = {291--302},
file = {Tassi et al_1992_Effects of noise on sleep inertia as a function of circadian placement of a.pdf:C\:\\Users\\Raphael\\AppData\\Roaming\\Mozilla\\Firefox\\Profiles\\mm1r2zj7.default-1442859820820\\zotero\\storage\\KKZ2ZZUM\\Tassi et al_1992_Effects of noise on sleep inertia as a function of circadian placement of a.pdf:application/pdf}
}
@article{de_gennaro_recovery_2010,
title = {Recovery sleep after sleep deprivation almost completely abolishes dream recall},
volume = {206},
issn = {0166-4328},
url = {http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0166432809005610},
doi = {10.1016/j.bbr.2009.09.030},
abstract = {The study investigated the effect of one night of sleep deprivation on dream recall at morning awakening after recovery sleep. Forty healthy subjects were studied after adaptation (A) and baseline nights (B), and a recovery (R) night following 40 h of prolonged wakefulness. Parallel to the well-known recovery sleep changes (slow-wave sleep – SWS – rebound, decreased number of awakenings and of REM sleep amount), an almost complete abolition of dream recall was found, with an around 75\% decrease with respect to the adaptation and baseline nights. The number of dreams recalled by those subjects with successful recall (REC) did not significantly differ between nights. Moreover, gender and sleep stage at awakening did not affect either the proportion of REC subjects or the number of dreams recalled by REC subjects during each night. Finally, the drastic impairment of dream recall after R night was associated to a larger increase of SWS and a shorter REM sleep duration. We suggest that dream recall could have been impaired during R night because: (i) the lower number of spontaneous awakenings over the night reduced the contents available in memory as possible cues for the retrieval of dream experiences at morning; (ii) mental experiences, having been elaborated during SWS more than in the other nights, were less dreamlike (i.e., perceptually vivid and bizarre) and, thus less accessible at morning recall than those elaborated during the nights with a higher proportion of REM sleep; (iii) dream contents, as a peculiar type of episodic information, were less consolidated because of the lower effectiveness of declarative memory during recovery sleep.},
number = {2},
urldate = {2015-05-06},
journal = {Behavioural Brain Research},
author = {De Gennaro, Luigi and Marzano, Cristina and Moroni, Fabio and Curcio, Giuseppe and Ferrara, Michele and Cipolli, Carlo},
month = jan,
year = {2010},
keywords = {Arousal–retrieval model, Declarative memory, Dreaming, dream recall, Morning awakening, Sleep Deprivation},
pages = {293--298},
file = {De Gennaro et al_2010_Recovery sleep after sleep deprivation almost completely abolishes dream recall.pdf:C\:\\Users\\Raphael\\AppData\\Roaming\\Mozilla\\Firefox\\Profiles\\mm1r2zj7.default-1442859820820\\zotero\\storage\\AVMTCBD4\\De Gennaro et al_2010_Recovery sleep after sleep deprivation almost completely abolishes dream recall.pdf:application/pdf;ScienceDirect Snapshot:C\:\\Users\\Raphael\\AppData\\Roaming\\Mozilla\\Firefox\\Profiles\\mm1r2zj7.default-1442859820820\\zotero\\storage\\FC6RCGG5\\S0166432809005610.html:text/html}
}
@incollection{larson-prior_modulation_2011,
title = {Modulation of the brain’s functional network architecture in the transition from wake to sleep},
volume = {193},
isbn = {978-0-444-53839-0},
language = {en},
urldate = {2015-05-06},
booktitle = {Progress in {Brain} {Research}},
publisher = {Elsevier},
author = {Larson-Prior, Linda J. and Power, Jonathan D. and Vincent, Justin L. and Nolan, Tracy S. and Coalson, Rebecca S. and Zempel, John and Snyder, Abraham Z. and Schlaggar, Bradley L. and Raichle, Marcus E. and Petersen, Steven E.},
year = {2011},
pages = {277--294},
file = {Larson-Prior et al_2011_Modulation of the brain’s functional network architecture in the transition.pdf:C\:\\Users\\Raphael\\AppData\\Roaming\\Mozilla\\Firefox\\Profiles\\mm1r2zj7.default-1442859820820\\zotero\\storage\\H5XCN984\\Larson-Prior et al_2011_Modulation of the brain’s functional network architecture in the transition.pdf:application/pdf}
}
@article{schredl_dream_2010,
title = {Dream content analysis: {Basic} principles},
volume = {3},
shorttitle = {Dream content analysis},
url = {https://journals.ub.uni-heidelberg.de/index.php/IJoDR/article/view/474},
number = {1},
urldate = {2016-01-07},
journal = {International Journal of Dream Research},
author = {Schredl, Michael},
year = {2010},
pages = {65--73},
file = {Schredl_2010_Dream content analysis.pdf:C\:\\Users\\Raphael\\AppData\\Roaming\\Mozilla\\Firefox\\Profiles\\mm1r2zj7.default-1442859820820\\zotero\\storage\\VZD4F62N\\Schredl_2010_Dream content analysis.pdf:application/pdf}
}
@article{marzano_recalling_2011,
title = {Recalling and {Forgetting} {Dreams}: {Theta} and {Alpha} {Oscillations} during {Sleep} {Predict} {Subsequent} {Dream} {Recall}},
volume = {31},
url = {http://www.jneurosci.org/content/31/18/6674.abstract},
doi = {10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0412-11.2011},
abstract = {Under the assumption that dream recall is a peculiar form of declarative memory, we have hypothesized that (1) the encoding of dream contents during sleep should share some electrophysiological mechanisms with the encoding of episodic memories of the awake brain and (2) recalling a dream(s) after awakening from non-rapid eye movement (NREM) and rapid eye movement (REM) sleep should be associated with different brain oscillations. Here, we report that cortical brain oscillations of human sleep are predictive of successful dream recall. In particular, after morning awakening from REM sleep, a higher frontal 5–7 Hz (theta) activity was associated with successful dream recall. This finding mirrors the increase in frontal theta activity during successful encoding of episodic memories in wakefulness. Moreover, in keeping with the different EEG background, a different predictive relationship was found after awakening from stage 2 NREM sleep. Specifically, a lower 8–12 Hz (alpha) oscillatory activity of the right temporal area was associated with a successful dream recall. These findings provide the first evidence of univocal cortical electroencephalographic correlates of dream recall, suggesting that the neurophysiological mechanisms underlying the encoding and recall of episodic memories may remain the same across different states of consciousness.},
number = {18},
journal = {The Journal of Neuroscience},
author = {Marzano, Cristina and Ferrara, Michele and Mauro, Federica and Moroni, Fabio and Gorgoni, Maurizio and Tempesta, Daniela and Cipolli, Carlo and De Gennaro, Luigi},
year = {2011},
pages = {6674--6683},
file = {Marzano et al. - 2011 - Recalling and Forgetting Dreams Theta and Alpha O.pdf:C\:\\Users\\Raphael\\AppData\\Roaming\\Mozilla\\Firefox\\Profiles\\mm1r2zj7.default-1442859820820\\zotero\\storage\\UG76F97S\\Marzano et al. - 2011 - Recalling and Forgetting Dreams Theta and Alpha O.pdf:application/pdf}
}
@article{malinowski_memory_2014,
title = {Memory sources of dreams: the incorporation of autobiographical rather than episodic experiences},
volume = {23},
issn = {09621105},
shorttitle = {Memory sources of dreams},
url = {http://doi.wiley.com/10.1111/jsr.12134},
doi = {10.1111/jsr.12134},
language = {en},
number = {4},
urldate = {2015-05-06},
journal = {Journal of Sleep Research},
author = {Malinowski, Josie E. and Horton, Caroline L.},
month = aug,
year = {2014},
pages = {441--447},
file = {Malinowski_Horton_2014_Memory sources of dreams.pdf:C\:\\Users\\Raphael\\AppData\\Roaming\\Mozilla\\Firefox\\Profiles\\mm1r2zj7.default-1442859820820\\zotero\\storage\\TNV2WW49\\Malinowski_Horton_2014_Memory sources of dreams.pdf:application/pdf}
}
@article{cartwright_relation_2006,
title = {Relation of dreams to waking concerns},
volume = {141},
issn = {0165-1781},
url = {http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0165178105002581},
doi = {10.1016/j.psychres.2005.05.013},
abstract = {To test that dreams are influenced by the pre-sleep waking emotional concerns of the sleeper and have an effect on waking adaptation, 20 depressed and 10 control subjects, who were all going through a divorce, were enrolled in a repeated measures study lasting 5 months. A Current Concerns test was administered on three occasions before nights when every REM period was interrupted to record recalled mental content. The degree of waking concern about the ex-spouse correlated significantly with the number of dreams in which the former partner appeared as a dream character. Those who were in remission at the follow-up evaluation had a higher percentage of well-developed dreams than those who remained depressed. Dreams of the former spouse reported by those in remission differed from those who remained depressed in the expression of dream affect and in the within-dream linkage among units of associated memory material. Dreams of the former spouse that are reported by those who are not in remission lack affect and connection to other memories.},
number = {3},
urldate = {2015-05-06},
journal = {Psychiatry Research},
author = {Cartwright, Rosalind and Agargun, Mehmet Y. and Kirkby, Jennifer and Friedman, Julie Kabat},
month = mar,
year = {2006},
keywords = {Affect continuity, Current concerns, Depression, Dream incorporation},
pages = {261--270},
file = {Cartwright et al_2006_Relation of dreams to waking concerns.pdf:C\:\\Users\\Raphael\\AppData\\Roaming\\Mozilla\\Firefox\\Profiles\\mm1r2zj7.default-1442859820820\\zotero\\storage\\KRKXHWFJ\\Cartwright et al_2006_Relation of dreams to waking concerns.pdf:application/pdf;ScienceDirect Snapshot:C\:\\Users\\Raphael\\AppData\\Roaming\\Mozilla\\Firefox\\Profiles\\mm1r2zj7.default-1442859820820\\zotero\\storage\\UAFNK5GZ\\S0165178105002581.html:text/html}
}
@article{schredl_factors_2006,
title = {Factors {Affecting} the {Continuity} {Between} {Waking} and {Dreaming}: {Emotional} {Intensity} and {Emotional} {Tone} of the {Waking}-{Life} {Event}},
volume = {8},
copyright = {(c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved},
issn = {1302-1192(Print)},
shorttitle = {Factors {Affecting} the {Continuity} {Between} {Waking} and {Dreaming}},
abstract = {Many researchers are advocating the so-called "continuity hypothesis" of dreaming which simply states that dreams reflect waking-life experiences. For deriving specific hypotheses, Schredl (2003) formulated a mathematical model that specifies factors that affect the probability that certain waking-life experiences are incorporated into subsequent dreams. The findings of the present diary study indicate that emotional intensity but not emotional tone of the waking-life events affects the incorporation into subsequent dreams. It seems very promising to investigate factors that affect the continuity between waking and dreaming with different methodological paradigms in order to arrive at a comprehensive, empirically tested, and precise continuity hypothesis.},
number = {1},
journal = {Sleep and Hypnosis},
author = {Schredl, Michael},
year = {2006},
keywords = {*Dreaming, *Emotional States, *Life Experiences, *Probability, Wakefulness},
pages = {1--5},
file = {APA PsycNET Snapshot:C\:\\Users\\Raphael\\AppData\\Roaming\\Mozilla\\Firefox\\Profiles\\mm1r2zj7.default-1442859820820\\zotero\\storage\\TIBHWF5W\\2006-08953-001.html:text/html;Schredl_2006_Factors Affecting the Continuity Between Waking and Dreaming.pdf:C\:\\Users\\Raphael\\AppData\\Roaming\\Mozilla\\Firefox\\Profiles\\mm1r2zj7.default-1442859820820\\zotero\\storage\\MUWPKZFH\\Schredl_2006_Factors Affecting the Continuity Between Waking and Dreaming.pdf:application/pdf}
}
@article{hirshkowitz_normal_2004,
title = {Normal human sleep: an overview},
volume = {88},
issn = {00257125},
shorttitle = {Normal human sleep},
url = {http://linkinghub.elsevier.com/retrieve/pii/S0025712504000173},
doi = {10.1016/j.mcna.2004.01.001},
language = {en},
number = {3},
urldate = {2015-05-06},
journal = {Medical Clinics of North America},
author = {Hirshkowitz, Max},
month = may,
year = {2004},
pages = {551--565},
file = {Hirshkowitz_2004_Normal human sleep.pdf:C\:\\Users\\Raphael\\AppData\\Roaming\\Mozilla\\Firefox\\Profiles\\mm1r2zj7.default-1442859820820\\zotero\\storage\\UWIIMKTM\\Hirshkowitz_2004_Normal human sleep.pdf:application/pdf}
}
@article{balkin_process_2002,
title = {The process of awakening: a {PET} study of regional brain activity patterns mediating the re-establishment of alertness and consciousness},
volume = {125},
issn = {0006-8950},
shorttitle = {The process of awakening},
abstract = {Awakening from sleep entails rapid re-establishment of consciousness followed by the relatively slow (20-30 min later) re-establishment of alertness--a temporal dissociation that facilitates specification of the physiological underpinnings of each of these facets of the awakening process. H(2)(15)O PET was used to assess changes in regional cerebral blood flow (rCBF) upon awakening from stage 2 sleep. Cerebral blood flow (CBF) was most rapidly re-established in centrencephalic regions (e.g. brainstem and thalamus), suggesting that the reactivation of these regions underlies the re-establishment of conscious awareness. Across the ensuing 15 min of wakefulness, further increases in CBF were evident primarily in anterior cortical regions, suggesting that the dissipation of sleep inertia effects (post-awakening performance and alertness deficits) is effected by reactivation of these regions. Concomitant shifts in correlation patterns of regional brain activity across the post-awakening period [in particular, a waning negative correlation between prefrontal cortex and mesencephalic reticular formation (RF) activity, and a waxing positive correlation between prefrontal cortex and ventromedial caudate nucleus (CAUD) activity] suggest that the post-awakening reversal of sleep inertia effects may be mediated by more than mere reactivation--it may also involve the functional reorganization of brain activity. Conversely, stable post-awakening correlations--such as those found between the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) and most other brain regions--may denote the pattern of functional connectivity that underlies consciousness itself.},
language = {eng},
number = {Pt 10},
journal = {Brain: A Journal of Neurology},
author = {Balkin, Thomas J. and Braun, Allen R. and Wesensten, Nancy J. and Jeffries, Keith and Varga, Mary and Baldwin, Paul and Belenky, Gregory and Herscovitch, Peter},
month = oct,
year = {2002},
pmid = {12244087},
keywords = {Adult, Brain, Brain Mapping, Cerebrovascular Circulation, Consciousness, Humans, Male, Sleep Stages, Tomography, Emission-Computed, Wakefulness},
pages = {2308--2319},
file = {Balkin et al_2002_The process of awakening.pdf:C\:\\Users\\Raphael\\AppData\\Roaming\\Mozilla\\Firefox\\Profiles\\mm1r2zj7.default-1442859820820\\zotero\\storage\\NS688K7A\\Balkin et al_2002_The process of awakening.pdf:application/pdf}
}
@article{marzano_electroencephalographic_2011,
title = {Electroencephalographic sleep inertia of the awakening brain},
volume = {176},
issn = {0306-4522},
url = {http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0306452210015915},
doi = {10.1016/j.neuroscience.2010.12.014},
abstract = {Sleep inertia (SI) denotes a period of hypovigilance, confusion and impaired cognitive and behavioral performance that immediately follows awakening. Based on the observation that the reactivation of some cortical areas is faster than other upon awakening, here we examined regional differences between presleep and postsleep waking period. Moreover, we also compared rapid eye movements (REM) and stage 2 non-rapid eye movements (NREM) awakenings in a within-subject design. Presleep and postsleep waking electroencephalogram (EEG; 5 min with eyes-closed and 5 min with eyes-open) of 18 healthy subjects (12 males, mean age=23.8±2.3 years) were recorded from 19 derivations. Participants slept for two consecutive nights in the laboratory. In one night they were awakened from stage 2 NREM, while in the other from REM sleep. EEG power spectra were calculated across the following bands: delta (1–4 Hz), theta (5–7 Hz), alpha (8–12 Hz), beta-1 (13–16 Hz) and beta-2 (17–24 Hz). Moreover, a detailed hertz-by-hertz analysis has been repeated in the 2–4 Hz frequency range. Postsleep wakefulness, compared to presleep, is characterized by a generalized decrease of higher beta-1 and beta-2 EEG power over almost all scalp locations. A detailed analysis of topographical modifications in the low-frequency range showed that postsleep wakefulness is characterized by an increased delta activity in the posterior scalp locations, and by a concomitant frontal decrease compared to presleep. Moreover, it was found a prevalence of EEG power in the high frequency ranges (beta-1 and beta-2) upon awakening from stage 2 compared to REM awakenings over the left anterior derivations. Altogether these findings support the hypothesis that a generalized reduction in beta activity and increased delta activity in more posterior areas upon awakening may represent the EEG substratum of the sleep inertia phenomenon.},
urldate = {2015-05-06},
journal = {Neuroscience},
author = {Marzano, Cristina and Ferrara, Michele and Moroni, F. and De Gennaro, L.},
month = mar,
year = {2011},
keywords = {awakening, cortical topography, EEG power, local EEG changes, sleep inertia},
pages = {308--317},
file = {Marzano et al_2011_Electroencephalographic sleep inertia of the awakening brain.pdf:C\:\\Users\\Raphael\\AppData\\Roaming\\Mozilla\\Firefox\\Profiles\\mm1r2zj7.default-1442859820820\\zotero\\storage\\JT4SG8Z6\\Marzano et al_2011_Electroencephalographic sleep inertia of the awakening brain.pdf:application/pdf;ScienceDirect Snapshot:C\:\\Users\\Raphael\\AppData\\Roaming\\Mozilla\\Firefox\\Profiles\\mm1r2zj7.default-1442859820820\\zotero\\storage\\SDNHIVAK\\S0306452210015915.html:text/html}
}
@article{eichenlaub_brain_2014,
title = {Brain {Reactivity} {Differentiates} {Subjects} with {High} and {Low} {Dream} {Recall} {Frequencies} during {Both} {Sleep} and {Wakefulness}},
volume = {24},
issn = {1047-3211, 1460-2199},
url = {http://www.cercor.oxfordjournals.org/cgi/doi/10.1093/cercor/bhs388},
doi = {10.1093/cercor/bhs388},
language = {en},
number = {5},
urldate = {2015-05-06},
journal = {Cerebral Cortex},
author = {Eichenlaub, Jean-Baptiste and Bertrand, Olivier and Morlet, Dominique and Ruby, Perrine},
month = may,
year = {2014},
keywords = {Acoustic Stimulation, Adult, Brain, Dreams, Electroencephalography, Electromyography, Evoked Potentials, Female, Humans, Male, Mental Recall, Neuropsychological Tests, Psychoacoustics, Sleep, Wakefulness, Young Adult},
pages = {1206--1215},
file = {Eichenlaub et al_2014_Brain Reactivity Differentiates Subjects with High and Low Dream Recall.pdf:C\:\\Users\\Raphael\\AppData\\Roaming\\Mozilla\\Firefox\\Profiles\\mm1r2zj7.default-1442859820820\\zotero\\storage\\G4M3DN6F\\Eichenlaub et al_2014_Brain Reactivity Differentiates Subjects with High and Low Dream Recall.pdf:application/pdf;Eichenlaub et al_2014_Brain Reactivity Differentiates Subjects with High and Low Dream Recall.pdf:C\:\\Users\\Raphael\\AppData\\Roaming\\Mozilla\\Firefox\\Profiles\\mm1r2zj7.default-1442859820820\\zotero\\storage\\ZQC7FIP4\\Eichenlaub et al_2014_Brain Reactivity Differentiates Subjects with High and Low Dream Recall.pdf:application/pdf}
}
@article{wu_variations_2012,
title = {Variations in {Connectivity} in the {Sensorimotor} and {Default}-{Mode} {Networks} {During} the {First} {Nocturnal} {Sleep} {Cycle}},
volume = {2},
issn = {2158-0014, 2158-0022},
url = {http://online.liebertpub.com/doi/abs/10.1089/brain.2012.0075},
doi = {10.1089/brain.2012.0075},
language = {en},
number = {4},
urldate = {2015-05-06},
journal = {Brain Connectivity},
author = {Wu, Changwei W. and Liu, Po-Yu and Tsai, Pei-Jung and Wu, Yu-Chin and Hung, Ching-Sui and Tsai, Yu-Che and Cho, Kuan-Hung and Biswal, Bharat B. and Chen, Chia-Ju and Lin, Ching-Po},
month = aug,
year = {2012},
pages = {177--190},
annote = {Annotations extraites (30/07/2016 à 18:34:23)"functional connectivity in both networks showed increasing dissociation as NREM sleep deepened, whereas hyperconnectivity occurred during rapid eye movement (REM) sleep" (Wu et al 2012:177)"compared with connectivity before sleep, the DMN presented a comparable connectivity pattern immediately after awakening" (Wu et al 2012:177)"whereas the connectivity of the sensorimotor network remained disrupted." (Wu et al 2012:177)"After the person awakened, the DMN connectivity was re-established before the sensorimotor reconnection" (Wu et al 2012:177)},
file = {Wu et al_2012_Variations in Connectivity in the Sensorimotor and Default-Mode Networks During.pdf:C\:\\Users\\Raphael\\AppData\\Roaming\\Mozilla\\Firefox\\Profiles\\mm1r2zj7.default-1442859820820\\zotero\\storage\\2ZDK23JN\\Wu et al_2012_Variations in Connectivity in the Sensorimotor and Default-Mode Networks During.pdf:application/pdf}
}
@article{edwards_dreaming_2013,
title = {Dreaming and insight},
volume = {4},
issn = {1664-1078},
url = {http://journal.frontiersin.org/article/10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00979/abstract},
doi = {10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00979},
urldate = {2015-05-06},
journal = {Frontiers in Psychology},
author = {Edwards, Christopher L. and Ruby, Perrine M. and Malinowski, Josie E. and Bennett, Paul D. and Blagrove, Mark T.},
year = {2013},
file = {Edwards et al_2013_Dreaming and insight.pdf:C\:\\Users\\Raphael\\AppData\\Roaming\\Mozilla\\Firefox\\Profiles\\mm1r2zj7.default-1442859820820\\zotero\\storage\\PI8GK3NH\\Edwards et al_2013_Dreaming and insight.pdf:application/pdf}
}
@article{whitfield-gabrieli_conn:_2012,
title = {Conn: a functional connectivity toolbox for correlated and anticorrelated brain networks},
volume = {2},
issn = {2158-0022},
shorttitle = {Conn},
doi = {10.1089/brain.2012.0073},
abstract = {Resting state functional connectivity reveals intrinsic, spontaneous networks that elucidate the functional architecture of the human brain. However, valid statistical analysis used to identify such networks must address sources of noise in order to avoid possible confounds such as spurious correlations based on non-neuronal sources. We have developed a functional connectivity toolbox Conn ( www.nitrc.org/projects/conn ) that implements the component-based noise correction method (CompCor) strategy for physiological and other noise source reduction, additional removal of movement, and temporal covariates, temporal filtering and windowing of the residual blood oxygen level-dependent (BOLD) contrast signal, first-level estimation of multiple standard functional connectivity magnetic resonance imaging (fcMRI) measures, and second-level random-effect analysis for resting state as well as task-related data. Compared to methods that rely on global signal regression, the CompCor noise reduction method allows for interpretation of anticorrelations as there is no regression of the global signal. The toolbox implements fcMRI measures, such as estimation of seed-to-voxel and region of interest (ROI)-to-ROI functional correlations, as well as semipartial correlation and bivariate/multivariate regression analysis for multiple ROI sources, graph theoretical analysis, and novel voxel-to-voxel analysis of functional connectivity. We describe the methods implemented in the Conn toolbox for the analysis of fcMRI data, together with examples of use and interscan reliability estimates of all the implemented fcMRI measures. The results indicate that the CompCor method increases the sensitivity and selectivity of fcMRI analysis, and show a high degree of interscan reliability for many fcMRI measures.},
language = {eng},
number = {3},
journal = {Brain Connectivity},
author = {Whitfield-Gabrieli, Susan and Nieto-Castanon, Alfonso},
year = {2012},
pmid = {22642651},
keywords = {Brain, Brain Mapping, Functional Neuroimaging, Humans, Magnetic Resonance Imaging, Mental Processes, Neural Pathways},
pages = {125--141},
annote = {Annotations extraites (30/07/2016 à 18:34:40)"In seed-driven RSFC analysis, Pearson's correlation coefficients are calculated between the seed time course and the time course of all other voxels, after which correlation coefficients are typically converted to normally distributed scores using Fisher's transform to allow for second-level General Linear Model analysis." (Whitfield-Gabrieli and Nieto-Castanon 2012:126)"In functional connectivity analysis, it is critical to appropriately address noise in order to avoid possible confounding effects (spurious correlations based on non-neuronal sources)." (Whitfield-Gabrieli and Nieto-Castanon 2012:126)"In Chai et al. (2012) we showed how a method for reducing spurious sources of variance in BOLD and perfusion-based fMRI, the anatomical component-based noise correction method (aCompCor) (Behzadi et al., 2007), can be particularly useful in the context of fcMRI analysis, increasing not only the validity, but also the sensitivity and specificity of these analyses." (Whitfield-Gabrieli and Nieto-Castanon 2012:126)"It models the influence of noise as a voxel-specific linear combination of multiple empirically estimated noise sources, which are estimated from the variability in BOLD responses within noise ROIs. This is particularly appropriate for fMRI noise sources as cardiac and respiratory effects do not have a common spatial distribution in their effects (e.g., cardiac effects are particularly visible near vessels and respiratory effects appear more globally and stronger near edges in the image)" (Whitfield-Gabrieli and Nieto-Castanon 2012:126)"tivit" (Whitfield-Gabrieli and Nieto-Castanon 2012:126)"The combination of the Conn's implementation of the CompCor method of noise reduction along with the efficient rejection of motion and artifactual time points allows for better interpretation of functional connectivity results for both correlated and anticorrelated networks." (Whitfield-Gabrieli and Nieto-Castanon 2012:126)"including spatial preprocessing of BOLD signal and anatomical volumes, CompCor removal of noise sources, first-level estimation of fcMRI measures, and second-level random effect analysis—while maintaining the flexibility to define and estimate different forms of fcMRI analysis" (Whitfield-Gabrieli and Nieto-Castanon 2012:126)Figure pour thèse ? (note on p.127)"The toolbox implements an anatomical aCompCor strategy (Behzadi et al., 2007) in which a userdefined number of orthogonal time series are estimated using principal component analysis (PCA) of the multivariate BOLD signal within each of these noise ROIs" (Whitfield-Gabrieli and Nieto-Castanon 2012:127)"Before being entered into second-level between-subjects analysis, a Fisher transformation (inverse hyperbolic tangent function) is applied to all bivariate and semipartial correlation measures in order to improve the normality assumptions of standard second-level general linear models." (Whitfield-Gabrieli and Nieto-Castanon 2012:129)"To take into account the hemodynamic delay, block regressors for each condition are convolved with a canonical hemodynamic response function, a combination of two gamma functions, and rectified \{filtered = to keep the positive part of the original time series; y[n] max(0,x[n])\}." (Whitfield-Gabrieli and Nieto-Castanon 2012:129)"Uncorrected p-values are appropriate when the researcher's original hypotheses involve only the connectivity between two a priori ROIs and FDR-corrected p-values are appropriate when the researcher's original hypotheses involve the connectivity between larger sets of ROIs and do not specify a priori which ROIs are expected to show an effect" (Whitfield-Gabrieli and Nieto-Castanon 2012:130)"The toolbox also offers a complete batch processing environment facilitating the implementation of scalable and robust functional connectivity analysis using a simple common framework" (Whitfield-Gabrieli and Nieto-Castanon 2012:136)"ysis package of choice for additional second-level analysis For example, researchers may enter the subject-level Z-maps (Fisher-transformed subject-level correlation coefficients when performing bivariate-correlation analysis) in their anal-" (Whitfield-Gabrieli and Nieto-Castanon 2012:136)"There is general agreement, however, as may be illustrated with mathematical proof, that a seed voxel analysis using global signal regression will necessarily show anticorrelations even if none were truly present in the data because after global regression the distribution of the correlation coefficients between a voxel and every other voxel in the £ brain is shifted such that the sum 0" (Whitfield-Gabrieli and Nieto-Castanon 2012:137)},
file = {Whitfield-Gabrieli_Nieto-Castanon_2012_Conn.pdf:C\:\\Users\\Raphael\\AppData\\Roaming\\Mozilla\\Firefox\\Profiles\\mm1r2zj7.default-1442859820820\\zotero\\storage\\9PXWUQ37\\Whitfield-Gabrieli_Nieto-Castanon_2012_Conn.pdf:application/pdf}
}
@article{ruby_alpha_2013,
title = {Alpha reactivity to first names differs in subjects with high and low dream recall frequency},
volume = {4},
issn = {1664-1078},
url = {http://journal.frontiersin.org/article/10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00419/abstract},
doi = {10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00419},
urldate = {2015-05-06},
journal = {Frontiers in Psychology},
author = {Ruby, Perrine and Blochet, Camille and Eichenlaub, Jean-Baptiste and Bertrand, Olivier and Morlet, Dominique and Bidet-Caulet, Aurélie},
year = {2013},
file = {Ruby et al_2013_Alpha reactivity to first names differs in subjects with high and low dream.PDF:C\:\\Users\\Raphael\\AppData\\Roaming\\Mozilla\\Firefox\\Profiles\\mm1r2zj7.default-1442859820820\\zotero\\storage\\GRQSIKI2\\Ruby et al_2013_Alpha reactivity to first names differs in subjects with high and low dream.PDF:application/pdf;Ruby et al_2013_Alpha reactivity to first names differs in subjects with high and low dream.pdf:C\:\\Users\\Raphael\\AppData\\Roaming\\Mozilla\\Firefox\\Profiles\\mm1r2zj7.default-1442859820820\\zotero\\storage\\T43HUHFX\\Ruby et al_2013_Alpha reactivity to first names differs in subjects with high and low dream.pdf:application/pdf}
}
@article{bastuji_laser_2008,
title = {Laser evoked responses to painful stimulation persist during sleep and predict subsequent arousals},
volume = {137},
issn = {1872-6623},
doi = {10.1016/j.pain.2007.10.027},
abstract = {We studied behavioural responses and 32-channel brain potentials to nociceptive stimuli during all-night sleep in 12 healthy subjects, using sequences of thermal laser pulses delivered over the dorsum of the hand. Laser stimuli less than 20 dB over perception threshold had clear awakening properties, in accordance with the intrinsic threatening value of nociceptive signals. Even in cases where nociceptive stimulation did not interrupt sleep, it triggered motor responses in 11\% of trials. Only four subjects reported dreams, and on morning questionnaires there was no evidence of incorporation to dreams of nociceptive stimuli. Contrary to previous reports suggesting the absence of cortical nociceptive responses during sleep, we were able to record brain-evoked potentials to laser (LEPs) during all sleep stages. Sleep LEPs were in general attenuated, but their morphology was sleep-stage-dependent: in stage 2, the weakened initial response was often followed by a high-amplitude negative wave with typical features of a K-complex. During paradoxical sleep (PS) LEP morphology was similar to that of waking, but frontal components showed strong attenuation, consistent with the reported frontal metabolic deactivation. A late positive component (450-650 ms) was recorded in both stage 2 and PS, the amplitude of which was significantly enhanced in trials that were followed by an arousal. This response appeared functionally related to the P3 wave, which in waking subjects has been associated to conscious perception and memory encoding.},
language = {eng},
number = {3},
journal = {Pain},
author = {Bastuji, H. and Perchet, C. and Legrain, V. and Montes, C. and Garcia-Larrea, L.},
month = jul,
year = {2008},
pmid = {18063478},
keywords = {Adult, Arousal, Evoked Potentials, Somatosensory, Female, Humans, Lasers, Male, Pain, Sleep},
pages = {589--599},
file = {Bastuji et al_2008_Laser evoked responses to painful stimulation persist during sleep and predict.pdf:C\:\\Users\\Raphael\\AppData\\Roaming\\Mozilla\\Firefox\\Profiles\\mm1r2zj7.default-1442859820820\\zotero\\storage\\V6Z3TA67\\Bastuji et al_2008_Laser evoked responses to painful stimulation persist during sleep and predict.pdf:application/pdf}
}
@incollection{schredl_characteristics_2010,
title = {Characteristics {And} {Contents} {Of} {Dreams}},
volume = {92},
url = {http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0074774210920072},
abstract = {Dreams have been studied from different perspectives: psychoanalysis, academic psychology, and neurosciences. After presenting the definition of dreaming and the methodological tools of dream research, the major findings regarding the phenomenology of dreaming and the factors influencing dream content are briefly reviewed. The so-called continuity hypothesis stating that dreams reflect waking-life experiences is supported by studies investigating the dreams of psychiatric patients and patients with sleep disorders, i.e., their daytime symptoms and problems are reflected in their dreams. Dreams also have an effect on subsequent waking life, e.g., on daytime mood and creativity. The question about the functions of dreaming is still unanswered and open to future research.},
urldate = {2015-05-06},
booktitle = {International {Review} of {Neurobiology}},
publisher = {Academic Press},
author = {Schredl, Michael},
editor = {McNamara, Angela Clow {and} Patrick},
year = {2010},
keywords = {Continuity Hypothesis, Dream content, dream content analysis, dream recall, mental disorders, Sleep Disorders},
pages = {135--154},
file = {Schredl_2010_Characteristics And Contents Of Dreams.pdf:C\:\\Users\\Raphael\\AppData\\Roaming\\Mozilla\\Firefox\\Profiles\\mm1r2zj7.default-1442859820820\\zotero\\storage\\GFHESZ2B\\Schredl_2010_Characteristics And Contents Of Dreams.pdf:application/pdf;ScienceDirect Snapshot:C\:\\Users\\Raphael\\AppData\\Roaming\\Mozilla\\Firefox\\Profiles\\mm1r2zj7.default-1442859820820\\zotero\\storage\\9M56IRUU\\S0074774210920072.html:text/html}
}
@article{esposito_reduced_2004,
title = {Reduced {Alpha} power associated with the recall of mentation from {Stage} 2 and {Stage} {REM} sleep},
volume = {41},
issn = {0048-5772, 1469-8986},
url = {http://doi.wiley.com/10.1111/j.1469-8986.00143.x},
doi = {10.1111/j.1469-8986.00143.x},
language = {en},
number = {2},
urldate = {2015-05-06},
journal = {Psychophysiology},
author = {Esposito, Maria Jose and Nielsen, Tore A. and Paquette, Tyna},
month = mar,
year = {2004},
pages = {288--297},
file = {Esposito et al_2004_Reduced Alpha power associated with the recall of mentation from Stage 2 and.pdf:C\:\\Users\\Raphael\\AppData\\Roaming\\Mozilla\\Firefox\\Profiles\\mm1r2zj7.default-1442859820820\\zotero\\storage\\EJHU2ZKR\\Esposito et al_2004_Reduced Alpha power associated with the recall of mentation from Stage 2 and.pdf:application/pdf}
}
@article{dresler_neural_2012,
title = {Neural correlates of dream lucidity obtained from contrasting lucid versus non-lucid {REM} sleep: a combined {EEG}/{fMRI} case study},
volume = {35},
issn = {1550-9109},
shorttitle = {Neural correlates of dream lucidity obtained from contrasting lucid versus non-lucid {REM} sleep},
doi = {10.5665/sleep.1974},
abstract = {STUDY OBJECTIVES: To investigate the neural correlates of lucid dreaming.
DESIGN: Parallel EEG/fMRI recordings of night sleep.
SETTING: Sleep laboratory and fMRI facilities.
PARTICIPANTS: Four experienced lucid dreamers.
INTERVENTIONS: N/A.
MEASUREMENTS AND RESULTS: Out of 4 participants, one subject had 2 episodes of verified lucid REM sleep of sufficient length to be analyzed by fMRI. During lucid dreaming the bilateral precuneus, cuneus, parietal lobules, and prefrontal and occipito-temporal cortices activated strongly as compared with non-lucid REM sleep.
CONCLUSIONS: In line with recent EEG data, lucid dreaming was associated with a reactivation of areas which are normally deactivated during REM sleep. This pattern of activity can explain the recovery of reflective cognitive capabilities that are the hallmark of lucid dreaming.},
language = {eng},
number = {7},
journal = {Sleep},
author = {Dresler, Martin and Wehrle, Renate and Spoormaker, Victor I. and Koch, Stefan P. and Holsboer, Florian and Steiger, Axel and Obrig, Hellmuth and Sämann, Philipp G. and Czisch, Michael},
month = jul,
year = {2012},
pmid = {22754049},
pmcid = {PMC3369221},
keywords = {Adult, Brain, Dreams, Electroencephalography, Functional Neuroimaging, Humans, Magnetic Resonance Imaging, Male, Polysomnography, Sleep, REM},
pages = {1017--1020},
file = {Dresler et al_2012_Neural correlates of dream lucidity obtained from contrasting lucid versus.pdf:C\:\\Users\\Raphael\\AppData\\Roaming\\Mozilla\\Firefox\\Profiles\\mm1r2zj7.default-1442859820820\\zotero\\storage\\8KA43VXD\\Dresler et al_2012_Neural correlates of dream lucidity obtained from contrasting lucid versus.pdf:application/pdf}
}
@article{eichenlaub_resting_2014,
title = {Resting brain activity varies with dream recall frequency between subjects},
volume = {39},
issn = {1740-634X},
doi = {10.1038/npp.2014.6},
abstract = {Dreaming is still poorly understood. Notably, its cerebral underpinning remains unclear. Neuropsychological studies have shown that lesions in the temporoparietal junction (TPJ) and/or the white matter of the medial prefrontal cortex (MPFC) lead to the global cessation of dream reports, suggesting that these regions of the default mode network have key roles in the dreaming process (forebrain 'dream-on' hypothesis). To test this hypothesis, we measured regional cerebral blood flow (rCBF) using [(15)O]H2O positron emission tomography in healthy subjects with high and low dream recall frequencies (DRFs) during wakefulness (rest) and sleep (rapid eye movement (REM) sleep, N2, and N3). Compared with Low recallers (0.5 ± 0.3 dream recall per week in average), High recallers (5.2 ± 1.4) showed higher rCBF in the TPJ during REM sleep, N3, and wakefulness, and in the MPFC during REM sleep and wakefulness. We demonstrate that the resting states of High recallers and Low recallers differ during sleep and wakefulness. It coheres with previous ERP results and confirms that a high/low DRF is associated with a specific functional organization of the brain. These results support the forebrain 'dream-on' hypothesis and suggest that TPJ and MPFC are not only involved in dream recall during wakefulness but also have a role in dreaming during sleep (production and/or encoding). Increased activity in the TPJ and MPFC might promote the mental imagery and/or memory encoding of dreams. Notably, increased activity in TPJ might facilitate attention orienting toward external stimuli and promote intrasleep wakefulness, facilitating the encoding of the dreams in memory.},
language = {eng},
number = {7},
journal = {Neuropsychopharmacology},
author = {Eichenlaub, Jean-Baptiste and Nicolas, Alain and Daltrozzo, Jérôme and Redouté, Jérôme and Costes, Nicolas and Ruby, Perrine},
month = jun,
year = {2014},
pmid = {24549103},
pmcid = {PMC4023156},
keywords = {Adolescent, Adult, Attention, Brain, Cerebrovascular Circulation, Dreams, Electroencephalography, Female, Functional Laterality, Humans, Magnetic Resonance Imaging, Male, Mental Recall, Oxygen Radioisotopes, Photic Stimulation, Positron-Emission Tomography, Rest, Sleep, Wakefulness, Young Adult},
pages = {1594--1602},
file = {Eichenlaub et al_2014_Resting brain activity varies with dream recall frequency between subjects.pdf:C\:\\Users\\Raphael\\AppData\\Roaming\\Mozilla\\Firefox\\Profiles\\mm1r2zj7.default-1442859820820\\zotero\\storage\\BZNGV98R\\Eichenlaub et al_2014_Resting brain activity varies with dream recall frequency between subjects.pdf:application/pdf;Eichenlaub et al_2014_Resting brain activity varies with dream recall frequency between subjects.pdf:C\:\\Users\\Raphael\\AppData\\Roaming\\Mozilla\\Firefox\\Profiles\\mm1r2zj7.default-1442859820820\\zotero\\storage\\UDGCZPG7\\Eichenlaub et al_2014_Resting brain activity varies with dream recall frequency between subjects.pdf:application/pdf}
}
@article{kaufmann_brain_2015,
title = {The brain functional connectome is robustly altered by lack of sleep},
issn = {10538119},
doi = {10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.12.028},
language = {en},
urldate = {2016-01-06},
journal = {NeuroImage},
author = {Kaufmann, Tobias and Elvsåshagen, Torbjørn and Alnæs, Dag and Zak, Nathalia and Pedersen, Per Ø. and Norbom, Linn B. and Quraishi, Sophia H. and Tagliazucchi, Enzo and Laufs, Helmut and Bjørnerud, Atle and Malt, Ulrik F. and Andreassen, Ole A. and Roussos, Evangelos and Duff, Eugene P. and Smith, Stephen M. and Groote, Inge R. and Westlye, Lars T.},
month = dec,
year = {2015},
file = {Kaufmann et al_2015_The brain functional connectome is robustly altered by lack of sleep.pdf:C\:\\Users\\Raphael\\AppData\\Roaming\\Mozilla\\Firefox\\Profiles\\mm1r2zj7.default-1442859820820\\zotero\\storage\\3UGKPZ4S\\Kaufmann et al_2015_The brain functional connectome is robustly altered by lack of sleep.pdf:application/pdf;Kaufmann et al_2015_The brain functional connectome is robustly altered by lack of sleep.pdf:C\:\\Users\\Raphael\\AppData\\Roaming\\Mozilla\\Firefox\\Profiles\\mm1r2zj7.default-1442859820820\\zotero\\storage\\FDE88AUT\\Kaufmann et al_2015_The brain functional connectome is robustly altered by lack of sleep.pdf:application/pdf}
}
@article{blagrove_distinguishing_2011,
title = {Distinguishing continuity/discontinuity, function and insight when investigating dream content},
volume = {4},
url = {http://journals.ub.uni-heidelberg.de/index.php/IJoDR/article/view/9153},
number = {2},
urldate = {2015-05-06},
journal = {International Journal of Dream Research},
author = {Blagrove, Mark},
year = {2011},
pages = {45--47},
file = {Blagrove_2011_Distinguishing continuity-discontinuity, function and insight when.pdf:C\:\\Users\\Raphael\\AppData\\Roaming\\Mozilla\\Firefox\\Profiles\\mm1r2zj7.default-1442859820820\\zotero\\storage\\QRIDQJFS\\Blagrove_2011_Distinguishing continuity-discontinuity, function and insight when.pdf:application/pdf}
}
@article{schredl_continuity_2003,
title = {Continuity between waking activities and dream activities},
volume = {12},
issn = {1053-8100},
url = {http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S1053810002000727},
doi = {10.1016/S1053-8100(02)00072-7},
abstract = {Empirical studies largely support the continuity hypothesis of dreaming. Despite of previous research efforts, the exact formulation of the continuity hypothesis remains vague. The present paper focuses on two aspects: (1) the differential incorporation rate of different waking-life activities and (2) the magnitude of which interindividual differences in waking-life activities are reflected in corresponding differences in dream content. Using a correlational design, a positive, non-zero correlation coefficient will support the continuity hypothesis. Although many researchers stress the importance of emotional involvement on the incorporation rate of waking-life experiences into dreams, Hartmann (2000) formulated the hypothesis that highly focused cognitive processes such as reading, writing, etc. are rarely found in dreams due to the cholinergic activation of the brain during dreaming. The present findings based on dream diaries and the exact measurement of waking activities replicated two recent questionnaire studies. These findings indicate that it will be necessary to specify the continuity hypothesis more fully and include factors (e.g., type of waking-life experience, emotional involvement) which modulate the incorporation rate of waking-life experiences into dreams. Whether the cholinergic state of the brain during REM sleep or other alterations of brain physiology (e.g., down-regulation of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex) are the underlying factors of the rare occurrence of highly focused cognitive processes in dreaming remains an open question. Although continuity between waking life and dreaming has been demonstrated, i.e., interindividual differences in the amount of time spent with specific waking-life activities are reflected in dream content, methodological issues (averaging over a two-week period, small number of dreams) have limited the capacity for detecting substantial relationships in all areas. Nevertheless, it might be concluded that the continuity hypothesis in its present general form is not valid and should be elaborated and tested in a more specific way.},
number = {2},
urldate = {2015-05-06},
journal = {Consciousness and Cognition},
author = {Schredl, Michael and Hofmann, Friedrich},
year = {2003},
keywords = {Continuity Hypothesis, Dream content},
pages = {298--308},
file = {Schredl_Hofmann_2003_Continuity between waking activities and dream activities.pdf:C\:\\Users\\Raphael\\AppData\\Roaming\\Mozilla\\Firefox\\Profiles\\mm1r2zj7.default-1442859820820\\zotero\\storage\\ITKZBRTW\\Schredl_Hofmann_2003_Continuity between waking activities and dream activities.pdf:application/pdf;ScienceDirect Snapshot:C\:\\Users\\Raphael\\AppData\\Roaming\\Mozilla\\Firefox\\Profiles\\mm1r2zj7.default-1442859820820\\zotero\\storage\\DPWKR8MJ\\S1053810002000727.html:text/html}
}
@article{de_gennaro_eeg_2001,
title = {{EEG} arousals in normal sleep: variations induced by total and selective slow-wave sleep deprivation},
volume = {24},
issn = {0161-8105},
shorttitle = {{EEG} arousals in normal sleep},
abstract = {STUDY OBJECTIVES: Aim of the present study was to assess changes in arousal rates after selective slow-wave (SWS) and total sleep deprivations.
DESIGN: Two-way mixed design comparing the arousal index (Al), as expressed by the number of EEG arousals divided by sleep duration, in totally or selectively sleep deprived subjects.
SETTING: Sleep laboratory.
PATIENTS OR PARTICIPANTS: Nineteen normal male subjects [mean age=23.3 years (S.E.M.=0.55)].
INTERVENTIONS: Al was measured in baseline nights and after selective SWS (N=10) and total sleep deprivation (N=9).
MEASUREMENTS AND RESULTS: During the baseline nights AI values changed across sleep stages as follows: stage 1 {\textgreater} stage 2 and REM {\textgreater} SWS, but did not present any significant variations as a function of time elapsed from sleep onset. The recovery after deprivation showed a reduction in EEG arousals, more pronounced after total sleep deprivation; this decrease affected NREM but not REM sleep. During the baseline nights Al showed a close-to-significance negative correlation with REM duration, while during the recovery nights a significant positive relation with stage 1 duration was found.
CONCLUSIONS: The present results suggest that recuperative processes after sleep deprivation are also associated with a higher sleep continuity as defined by the reduction of EEG arousals.},
language = {eng},
number = {6},
journal = {Sleep},
author = {De Gennaro, L. and Ferrara, Michele and Bertini, M.},
month = sep,
year = {2001},
pmid = {11560180},
keywords = {Adult, Arousal, Brain, Electroencephalography, Humans, Male, Polysomnography, Retrospective Studies, Sleep, Sleep Deprivation, Sleep, REM},
pages = {673--679},
file = {De Gennaro et al_2001_EEG arousals in normal sleep.pdf:C\:\\Users\\Raphael\\AppData\\Roaming\\Mozilla\\Firefox\\Profiles\\mm1r2zj7.default-1442859820820\\zotero\\storage\\BA344SMX\\De Gennaro et al_2001_EEG arousals in normal sleep.pdf:application/pdf}
}
@article{bonnet_scoring_2007,
title = {The scoring of arousal in sleep: reliability, validity, and alternatives},
volume = {3},
issn = {1550-9389},
shorttitle = {The scoring of arousal in sleep},
abstract = {The reliability and validity of EEG arousals and other types of arousal are reviewed. Brief arousals during sleep had been observed for many years, but the evolution of sleep medicine in the 1980s directed new attention to these events. Early studies at that time in animals and humans linked brief EEG arousals and associated fragmentation of sleep to daytime sleepiness and degraded performance. Increasing interest in scoring of EEG arousals led the ASDA to publish a scoring manual in 1992. The current review summarizes numerous studies that have examined scoring reliability for these EEG arousals. Validity of EEG arousals was explored by review of studies that empirically varied arousals and found deficits similar to those found after total sleep deprivation depending upon the rate and extent of sleep fragmentation. Additional data from patients with clinical sleep disorders prior to and after effective treatment has also shown a continuing relationship between reduction in pathology-related arousals and improved sleep and daytime function. Finally, many suggestions have been made to refine arousal scoring to include additional elements (e.g., CAP), change the time frame, or focus on other physiological responses such as heart rate or blood pressure changes. Evidence to support the reliability and validity of these measures is presented. It was concluded that the scoring of EEG arousals has added much to our understanding of the sleep process but that significant work on the neurophysiology of arousal needs to be done. Additional refinement of arousal scoring will provide improved insight into sleep pathology and recovery.},
language = {eng},
number = {2},
journal = {Journal of clinical sleep medicine: JCSM: official publication of the American Academy of Sleep Medicine},
author = {Bonnet, Michael H. and Doghramji, Karl and Roehrs, Timothy and Stepanski, Edward J. and Sheldon, Stephen H. and Walters, Arthur S. and Wise, Merrill and Chesson, Andrew L.},
month = mar,
year = {2007},
pmid = {17557423},
keywords = {Arousal, Electroencephalography, Humans, Oxygen, Reproducibility of Results, Research, Research Design, Sleep Apnea, Obstructive, Sleep Apnea Syndromes, Sleep Deprivation, Sleep Disorders},
pages = {133--145},
file = {Bonnet et al_2007_The scoring of arousal in sleep.pdf:C\:\\Users\\Raphael\\AppData\\Roaming\\Mozilla\\Firefox\\Profiles\\mm1r2zj7.default-1442859820820\\zotero\\storage\\KN8SSTX8\\Bonnet et al_2007_The scoring of arousal in sleep.pdf:application/pdf}
}
@article{berthomier_automatic_2007,
title = {Automatic analysis of single-channel sleep {EEG}: validation in healthy individuals},
volume = {30},
shorttitle = {Automatic analysis of single-channel sleep {EEG}},
url = {http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC2082104/},
number = {11},
urldate = {2015-05-06},
journal = {Sleep},
author = {Berthomier, Christian and Drouot, Xavier and Herman-Stoïca, Maria and Berthomier, Pierre and Prado, Jacques and Bokar-Thire, Djibril and Benoit, Odile and Mattout, Jérémie and d'Ortho, Marie-Pia},
year = {2007},
pages = {1587},
file = {Berthomier et al_2007_Automatic analysis of single-channel sleep EEG.pdf:C\:\\Users\\Raphael\\AppData\\Roaming\\Mozilla\\Firefox\\Profiles\\mm1r2zj7.default-1442859820820\\zotero\\storage\\UCXW8MZ9\\Berthomier et al_2007_Automatic analysis of single-channel sleep EEG.pdf:application/pdf}
}
@article{voss_lucid_2009,
title = {Lucid dreaming: a state of consciousness with features of both waking and non-lucid dreaming},
volume = {32},
shorttitle = {Lucid dreaming},
url = {http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/pmc2737577/},
number = {9},
urldate = {2015-05-06},
journal = {Sleep},
author = {Voss, Ursula and Holzmann, Romain and Tuin, Inka and Hobson, J. Allan},
year = {2009},
pages = {1191},
file = {Voss et al_2009_Lucid dreaming.pdf:C\:\\Users\\Raphael\\AppData\\Roaming\\Mozilla\\Firefox\\Profiles\\mm1r2zj7.default-1442859820820\\zotero\\storage\\R2KFMW2P\\Voss et al_2009_Lucid dreaming.pdf:application/pdf}
}
@article{van_rijn_dream-lag_2015,
title = {The dream-lag effect: {Selective} processing of personally significant events during {Rapid} {Eye} {Movement} sleep, but not during {Slow} {Wave} {Sleep}},
issn = {1095-9564},
shorttitle = {The dream-lag effect},
doi = {10.1016/j.nlm.2015.01.009},
abstract = {Incorporation of details from waking life events into Rapid Eye Movement (REM) sleep dreams has been found to be highest on the night after, and then 5-7 nights after events (termed, respectively, the day-residue and dream-lag effects). In experiment 1, 44 participants kept a daily log for 10days, reporting major daily activities (MDAs), personally significant events (PSEs), and major concerns (MCs). Dream reports were collected from REM and Slow Wave Sleep (SWS) in the laboratory, or from REM sleep at home. The dream-lag effect was found for the incorporation of PSEs into REM dreams collected at home, but not for MDAs or MCs. No dream-lag effect was found for SWS dreams, or for REM dreams collected in the lab after SWS awakenings earlier in the night. In experiment 2, the 44 participants recorded reports of their spontaneously recalled home dreams over the 10 nights following the instrumental awakenings night, which thus acted as a controlled stimulus with two salience levels, high (sleep lab) and low (home awakenings). The dream-lag effect was found for the incorporation into home dreams of references to the experience of being in the sleep laboratory, but only for participants who had reported concerns beforehand about being in the sleep laboratory. The delayed incorporation of events from daily life into dreams has been proposed to reflect REM sleep-dependent memory consolidation. However, an alternative emotion processing or emotional impact of events account, distinct from memory consolidation, is supported by the finding that SWS dreams do not evidence the dream-lag effect.},
language = {ENG},
journal = {Neurobiology of Learning and Memory},
author = {van Rijn, E. and Eichenlaub, J.-B. and Lewis, P. A. and Walker, M. P. and Gaskell, M. G. and Malinowski, J. E. and Blagrove, M.},
month = feb,
year = {2015},
pmid = {25683202}
}
@article{bonnet_eeg_2007,
title = {{EEG} arousal norms by age},
volume = {3},
issn = {1550-9389},
abstract = {STUDY OBJECTIVES: Brief arousals have been systematically scored during sleep for more than 20 years. Despite significant knowledge concerning the importance of arousals for the sleep process in normal subjects and patients, comprehensive age norms have not been published.
METHODS: Seventy-six normal subjects (40 men) without sleep apnea or periodic limb movements of sleep, aged 18 to 70 years, slept in the sleep laboratory for 1 or more nights. Sleep and arousal data were scored by the same scorer for the first night (comparable to clinical polysomnograms) and summarized by age decade.
RESULTS: There were no statistically significant differences for sex or interaction of sex by age (p {\textgreater} .5 for both). The mean arousal index increased as a function of age. Newman-Keuls comparisons (.05) showed arousal index in the 18- to 20-year and 21- to 30-year age groups to be significantly less than the arousal index in the other 4 age groups. Arousal index in the 31-to 40-year and 41-to 50-year groups was significantly less than the arousal index in the older groups. The arousal index was significantly negatively correlated with total sleep time and all sleep stages (positive correlation with stage 1 and wake).
CONCLUSIONS: Brief arousals are an integral component of the sleep process. They increase with other electroencephalographic markers as a function of age. They are highly correlated with traditional sleep-stage amounts and are related to major demographic variables. Age-related norms may make identification of pathologic arousal easier.},
language = {eng},
number = {3},
journal = {Journal of clinical sleep medicine: JCSM: official publication of the American Academy of Sleep Medicine},
author = {Bonnet, Michael H. and Arand, Donna L.},
month = apr,
year = {2007},
pmid = {17561594},
pmcid = {PMC2564772},
keywords = {Adolescent, Aged, Age Factors, Aging, Apnea, Arousal, Electroencephalography, Female, Humans, Male, Middle Aged, Polysomnography, Sleep, REM, Wakefulness},
pages = {271--274},
file = {Bonnet_Arand_2007_EEG arousal norms by age.pdf:C\:\\Users\\Raphael\\AppData\\Roaming\\Mozilla\\Firefox\\Profiles\\mm1r2zj7.default-1442859820820\\zotero\\storage\\D6FUA62B\\Bonnet_Arand_2007_EEG arousal norms by age.pdf:application/pdf}
}
@article{spaniol_event-related_2009,
title = {Event-related {fMRI} studies of episodic encoding and retrieval: {Meta}-analyses using activation likelihood estimation},
volume = {47},
issn = {00283932},
shorttitle = {Event-related {fMRI} studies of episodic encoding and retrieval},
url = {http://linkinghub.elsevier.com/retrieve/pii/S0028393209001067},
doi = {10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.02.028},
language = {en},
number = {8-9},
urldate = {2015-05-06},
journal = {Neuropsychologia},
author = {Spaniol, Julia and Davidson, Patrick S.R. and Kim, Alice S.N. and Han, Hua and Moscovitch, Morris and Grady, Cheryl L.},
month = jul,
year = {2009},
pages = {1765--1779},
file = {Spaniol et al_2009_Event-related fMRI studies of episodic encoding and retrieval.pdf:C\:\\Users\\Raphael\\AppData\\Roaming\\Mozilla\\Firefox\\Profiles\\mm1r2zj7.default-1442859820820\\zotero\\storage\\UE8C69TS\\Spaniol et al_2009_Event-related fMRI studies of episodic encoding and retrieval.pdf:application/pdf}
}
@article{nielsen_review_2000,
title = {A review of mentation in {REM} and {NREM} sleep:“covert” {REM} sleep as a possible reconciliation of two opposing models},
volume = {23},
shorttitle = {A review of mentation in {REM} and {NREM} sleep},
url = {http://journals.cambridge.org/abstract_S0140525X0000399X},
number = {06},
urldate = {2015-05-26},
journal = {Behavioral and Brain Sciences},
author = {Nielsen, Tore A.},
year = {2000},
pages = {851--866},
file = {Nielsen_2000_A review of mentation in REM and NREM sleep.pdf:C\:\\Users\\Raphael\\AppData\\Roaming\\Mozilla\\Firefox\\Profiles\\mm1r2zj7.default-1442859820820\\zotero\\storage\\FRFZZVBH\\Nielsen_2000_A review of mentation in REM and NREM sleep.pdf:application/pdf}
}
@article{schredl_factors_2003,
title = {Factors of home dream recall: a structural equation model},
volume = {12},
shorttitle = {Factors of home dream recall},
url = {http://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1046/j.1365-2869.2003.00344.x/full},
number = {2},
urldate = {2015-05-06},
journal = {Journal of sleep research},
author = {Schredl, Michael and Wittmann, Lutz and Ciric, Petra and GÖtz, Simon},
year = {2003},
pages = {133--141},
file = {Schredl et al_2003_Factors of home dream recall.pdf:C\:\\Users\\Raphael\\AppData\\Roaming\\Mozilla\\Firefox\\Profiles\\mm1r2zj7.default-1442859820820\\zotero\\storage\\KM34X785\\Schredl et al_2003_Factors of home dream recall.pdf:application/pdf}
}
@article{fosse_dreaming_2003,
title = {Dreaming and episodic memory: a functional dissociation?},
volume = {15},
shorttitle = {Dreaming and episodic memory},
url = {http://www.mitpressjournals.org/doi/abs/10.1162/089892903321107774},
number = {1},
urldate = {2016-01-19},
journal = {Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience},
author = {Fosse, Magdalena J. and Fosse, Roar and Hobson, J. Allan and Stickgold, Robert},
year = {2003},
pages = {1--9},
file = {Fosse et al_2003_Dreaming and episodic memory.pdf:C\:\\Users\\Raphael\\AppData\\Roaming\\Mozilla\\Firefox\\Profiles\\mm1r2zj7.default-1442859820820\\zotero\\storage\\T23KCBA6\\Fosse et al_2003_Dreaming and episodic memory.pdf:application/pdf}
}
@article{guenole_dreams_2013,
title = {Do dreams really guard sleep? {Evidence} for and against {Freud}'s theory of the basic function of dreaming},
volume = {4},
issn = {1664-1078},
shorttitle = {Do dreams really guard sleep?},
url = {http://journal.frontiersin.org/article/10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00017/abstract},
doi = {10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00017},
urldate = {2015-10-13},
journal = {Frontiers in Psychology},
author = {Guénolé, Fabian and Marcaggi, Geoffrey and Baleyte, Jean-Marc},
year = {2013},
file = {Guénolé et al_2013_Do dreams really guard sleep.pdf:C\:\\Users\\Raphael\\AppData\\Roaming\\Mozilla\\Firefox\\Profiles\\mm1r2zj7.default-1442859820820\\zotero\\storage\\624QIEJZ\\Guénolé et al_2013_Do dreams really guard sleep.pdf:application/pdf}
}
@article{blagrove_replication_2011,
title = {A replication of the 5–7day dream-lag effect with comparison of dreams to future events as control for baseline matching},
volume = {20},
issn = {10538100},
url = {http://linkinghub.elsevier.com/retrieve/pii/S1053810010001480},
doi = {10.1016/j.concog.2010.07.006},
language = {en},
number = {2},
urldate = {2015-10-15},
journal = {Consciousness and Cognition},
author = {Blagrove, Mark and Henley-Einion, Josie and Barnett, Amanda and Edwards, Darren and Heidi Seage, C.},
month = jun,
year = {2011},
keywords = {Day-residue, Dream content, Dreaming, Dream-lag, Functions of dreaming, Functions of sleep, Memory, Memory consolidation},
pages = {384--391},
file = {Blagrove et al_2011_A replication of the 5–7day dream-lag effect with comparison of dreams to.pdf:C\:\\Users\\Raphael\\AppData\\Roaming\\Mozilla\\Firefox\\Profiles\\mm1r2zj7.default-1442859820820\\zotero\\storage\\MD7NSMWR\\Blagrove et al_2011_A replication of the 5–7day dream-lag effect with comparison of dreams to.pdf:application/pdf;Blagrove et al_2011_A replication of the 5–7day dream-lag effect with comparison of dreams to.pdf:C\:\\Users\\Raphael\\AppData\\Roaming\\Mozilla\\Firefox\\Profiles\\mm1r2zj7.default-1442859820820\\zotero\\storage\\SGPS4SAA\\Blagrove et al_2011_A replication of the 5–7day dream-lag effect with comparison of dreams to.pdf:application/pdf;ScienceDirect Snapshot:C\:\\Users\\Raphael\\AppData\\Roaming\\Mozilla\\Firefox\\Profiles\\mm1r2zj7.default-1442859820820\\zotero\\storage\\JZDIKGBE\\S1053810010001480.html:text/html}
}
@incollection{blagrove_trait_2010,
title = {Trait {And} {Neurobiological} {Correlates} {Of} {Individual} {Differences} {In} {Dream} {Recall} {And} {Dream} {Content}},
volume = {92},
isbn = {978-0-12-381322-0},
url = {http://linkinghub.elsevier.com/retrieve/pii/S0074774210920084},
language = {en},
urldate = {2015-10-15},
booktitle = {International {Review} of {Neurobiology}},
publisher = {Elsevier},
author = {Blagrove, Mark and Pace-Schott, Edward F.},
year = {2010},
keywords = {dream, Dream content, dream recall, neurobiology, personality},
pages = {155--180},
file = {Blagrove_Pace-Schott_2010_Trait And Neurobiological Correlates Of Individual Differences In Dream Recall.pdf:C\:\\Users\\Raphael\\AppData\\Roaming\\Mozilla\\Firefox\\Profiles\\mm1r2zj7.default-1442859820820\\zotero\\storage\\5HMX8CW8\\Blagrove_Pace-Schott_2010_Trait And Neurobiological Correlates Of Individual Differences In Dream Recall.pdf:application/pdf;Blagrove_Pace-Schott_2010_Trait And Neurobiological Correlates Of Individual Differences In Dream Recall.pdf:C\:\\Users\\Raphael\\AppData\\Roaming\\Mozilla\\Firefox\\Profiles\\mm1r2zj7.default-1442859820820\\zotero\\storage\\URN6N3BN\\Blagrove_Pace-Schott_2010_Trait And Neurobiological Correlates Of Individual Differences In Dream Recall.pdf:application/pdf;ScienceDirect Snapshot:C\:\\Users\\Raphael\\AppData\\Roaming\\Mozilla\\Firefox\\Profiles\\mm1r2zj7.default-1442859820820\\zotero\\storage\\8GD2DU8B\\S0074774210920084.html:text/html}
}
@article{gorgoni_eeg_2015,
title = {{EEG} topography during sleep inertia upon awakening after a period of increased homeostatic sleep pressure},
volume = {16},
issn = {13899457},
url = {http://linkinghub.elsevier.com/retrieve/pii/S1389945715007005},
doi = {10.1016/j.sleep.2015.03.009},
language = {en},
number = {7},
urldate = {2016-03-15},
journal = {Sleep Medicine},
author = {Gorgoni, Maurizio and Ferrara, Michele and D'Atri, Aurora and Lauri, Giulia and Scarpelli, Serena and Truglia, Ilaria and De Gennaro, Luigi},
month = jul,
year = {2015},
pages = {883--890},
file = {Gorgoni et al_2015_EEG topography during sleep inertia upon awakening after a period of increased.pdf:C\:\\Users\\Raphael\\AppData\\Roaming\\Mozilla\\Firefox\\Profiles\\mm1r2zj7.default-1442859820820\\zotero\\storage\\9RWDP79W\\Gorgoni et al_2015_EEG topography during sleep inertia upon awakening after a period of increased.pdf:application/pdf}
}
@article{mazza_asleep_2014,
title = {Asleep but aware?},
volume = {87},
issn = {02782626},
url = {http://linkinghub.elsevier.com/retrieve/pii/S0278262614000335},
doi = {10.1016/j.bandc.2014.02.007},
language = {en},
urldate = {2015-12-04},
journal = {Brain and Cognition},
author = {Mazza, Stéphanie and Perchet, Caroline and Frot, Maud and Michael, George A. and Magnin, Michel and Garcia-Larrea, Luis and Bastuji, Hélène},
month = jun,
year = {2014},
pages = {7--15},
file = {Mazza et al_2014_Asleep but aware.pdf:C\:\\Users\\Raphael\\AppData\\Roaming\\Mozilla\\Firefox\\Profiles\\mm1r2zj7.default-1442859820820\\zotero\\storage\\Q3PF5ZRG\\Mazza et al_2014_Asleep but aware.pdf:application/pdf}
}
@article{malinowski_evidence_2014,
title = {Evidence for the preferential incorporation of emotional waking-life experiences into dreams.},
volume = {24},
issn = {1573-3351, 1053-0797},
url = {http://doi.apa.org/getdoi.cfm?doi=10.1037/a0036017},
doi = {10.1037/a0036017},
language = {en},
number = {1},
urldate = {2015-10-15},
journal = {Dreaming},
author = {Malinowski, Josie and Horton, Caroline L.},
year = {2014},
pages = {18--31},
file = {Malinowski_Horton_2014_Evidence for the preferential incorporation of emotional waking-life.pdf:C\:\\Users\\Raphael\\AppData\\Roaming\\Mozilla\\Firefox\\Profiles\\mm1r2zj7.default-1442859820820\\zotero\\storage\\MWBWVGG3\\Malinowski_Horton_2014_Evidence for the preferential incorporation of emotional waking-life.pdf:application/pdf}
}
@article{llewellyn_such_2013,
title = {Such stuff as dreams are made on? {Elaborative} encoding, the ancient art of memory, and the hippocampus},
volume = {36},
issn = {0140-525X, 1469-1825},
shorttitle = {Such stuff as dreams are made on?},
url = {http://www.journals.cambridge.org/abstract_S0140525X12003135},
doi = {10.1017/S0140525X12003135},
language = {en},
number = {06},
urldate = {2015-10-21},
journal = {Behavioral and Brain Sciences},
author = {Llewellyn, Sue},
month = dec,
year = {2013},
keywords = {Memory consolidation},
pages = {589--607},
file = {Llewellyn_2013_Such stuff as dreams are made on.pdf:C\:\\Users\\Raphael\\AppData\\Roaming\\Mozilla\\Firefox\\Profiles\\mm1r2zj7.default-1442859820820\\zotero\\storage\\4BA83KB3\\Llewellyn_2013_Such stuff as dreams are made on.pdf:application/pdf;Llewellyn_BBS13.pdf:C\:\\Users\\Raphael\\AppData\\Roaming\\Mozilla\\Firefox\\Profiles\\mm1r2zj7.default-1442859820820\\zotero\\storage\\34FAF38R\\Llewellyn_BBS13.pdf:application/pdf}
}
@article{wamsley_dreaming_2014,
title = {Dreaming and {Offline} {Memory} {Consolidation}},
volume = {14},
issn = {1528-4042, 1534-6293},
url = {http://link.springer.com/10.1007/s11910-013-0433-5},
doi = {10.1007/s11910-013-0433-5},
language = {en},
number = {3},
urldate = {2015-10-21},
journal = {Current Neurology and Neuroscience Reports},
author = {Wamsley, Erin J.},
month = mar,
year = {2014},
keywords = {Memory consolidation},
pages = {433},
annote = {Wamsley - 2014 - Dreaming and Offline memory consolidation - Review
Arguments pour :
Novel experiences have a particularly pronounced impact on dream experience.
Roffwarg et al. 68-74 : effect on wearing red-tinted googles for a long period influence dreams.
Dement et al. 1965 - 22\% of dream reports incorporated elements from the lab, which could represent an impactful event.
Fosse et al. 1977 : after reading the text of a short story, participants who reported dreams related to the story exhibited superior memory for the text the following morning --{\textgreater} Quel était le critère d'incorporation ? Test de mémoire ?
De Koninck et al. 1990 : among students enrolled in a french immersion class, those who incorporated the most French into their dreams were also the one with the best success rate. --{\textgreater} Encore une fois, quel était le critère d'incorporation ? et ça parle plus d'une corrélation mais pas d'une causalité !
Wamsley et al. 2010 - 2012 : dreaming of a virtual maze navigation task is associated with enhanced consolidation of spatial memory both across a nap (2010) and a full night of sleep (2012).
Arguments contre :
1) Dreams do not replicate exact waking life experiences (Vertes, 2004 ; Vertes, 2005 ; Hartmann 2010)
--{\textgreater} Pour Wamsley, sleep facilitates integration across multiple memories and the extraction of generalities. This intermingling of memory fragments seen in dream reports could well reflect this adaptative process.
2) Not all dreaming is related to a past memory: memory consolidation is only one major influence on dreaming but is not all the dream.
3) Dreams do not seem functional
Owen flanagan : "Since we rarely dream about what we need to remember, the hypothesis that dreams themselves serve any memory enhancing function appears unwarranted". (livre Dreaming souls, 1999)
--{\textgreater} Pour Wamsley il faut bien dissocier le processus du rêve, qui peut tout à fait ne pas être fonctionnel, et les processus de consolidation sous-jacents. Le reve peut refleter ces phénomènes sans toutefois être fonctionnel en lui même
La vraie question est donc plutôt de savoir si le sommeil a bel et bien un effet dans la consolidation mnésique !
},
file = {Wamsley_2014_Dreaming and Offline Memory Consolidation.pdf:C\:\\Users\\Raphael\\AppData\\Roaming\\Mozilla\\Firefox\\Profiles\\mm1r2zj7.default-1442859820820\\zotero\\storage\\TRFV9RG7\\Wamsley_2014_Dreaming and Offline Memory Consolidation.pdf:application/pdf}
}
@article{diekelmann_memory_2010,
title = {The memory function of sleep},
issn = {1471-003X, 1471-0048},
url = {http://www.nature.com/doifinder/10.1038/nrn2762},
doi = {10.1038/nrn2762},
urldate = {2015-10-23},
journal = {Nature Reviews Neuroscience},
author = {Diekelmann, Susanne and Born, Jan},
month = oct,
year = {2010},
file = {Diekelmann_Born_2010_The memory function of sleep.pdf:C\:\\Users\\Raphael\\AppData\\Roaming\\Mozilla\\Firefox\\Profiles\\mm1r2zj7.default-1442859820820\\zotero\\storage\\D7CCMRE4\\Diekelmann_Born_2010_The memory function of sleep.pdf:application/pdf}
}
@article{domhoff_dreaming_2015,
title = {Dreaming and the default network: {A} review, synthesis, and counterintuitive research proposal},
volume = {33},
issn = {10538100},
shorttitle = {Dreaming and the default network},
url = {http://linkinghub.elsevier.com/retrieve/pii/S1053810015000264},
doi = {10.1016/j.concog.2015.01.019},
language = {en},
urldate = {2015-10-28},
journal = {Consciousness and Cognition},
author = {Domhoff, G. William and Fox, Kieran C.R.},
month = may,
year = {2015},
keywords = {DMN, Resting state},
pages = {342--353},
file = {Domhoff_Fox_2015_Dreaming and the default network.pdf:C\:\\Users\\Raphael\\AppData\\Roaming\\Mozilla\\Firefox\\Profiles\\mm1r2zj7.default-1442859820820\\zotero\\storage\\CJM546B4\\Domhoff_Fox_2015_Dreaming and the default network.pdf:application/pdf}
}
@article{revonsuo_reinterpretation_2000,
title = {The reinterpretation of dreams: {An} evolutionary hypothesis of the function of dreaming},
volume = {23},
shorttitle = {The reinterpretation of dreams},
url = {http://journals.cambridge.org/abstract_S0140525X00004015},
number = {06},
urldate = {2015-10-30},
journal = {Behavioral and Brain Sciences},
author = {Revonsuo, Antti},
year = {2000},
keywords = {dream functions, Threat simulation theory},
pages = {877--901},
file = {Revonsuo_2000_The reinterpretation of dreams.pdf:C\:\\Users\\Raphael\\AppData\\Roaming\\Mozilla\\Firefox\\Profiles\\mm1r2zj7.default-1442859820820\\zotero\\storage\\QE8Z4EWN\\Revonsuo_2000_The reinterpretation of dreams.pdf:application/pdf}
}
@article{barrett__1993,
title = {The" committee of sleep": {A} study of dream incubation for problem solving.},
volume = {3},
shorttitle = {The" committee of sleep"},
url = {http://psycnet.apa.org/journals/drm/3/2/115/},
number = {2},
urldate = {2015-10-30},
journal = {Dreaming},
author = {Barrett, Deirdre},
year = {1993},
pages = {115--122},
file = {Barrett_1993_The committee of sleep.pdf:C\:\\Users\\Raphael\\AppData\\Roaming\\Mozilla\\Firefox\\Profiles\\mm1r2zj7.default-1442859820820\\zotero\\storage\\BQ3588C7\\Barrett_1993_The committee of sleep.pdf:application/pdf}
}
@article{bastuji_event-related_2003,
title = {Event-related potentials during forced awakening: a tool for the study of acute sleep inertia},
volume = {12},
shorttitle = {Event-related potentials during forced awakening},
url = {http://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1046/j.1365-2869.2003.00358.x/full},
number = {3},
urldate = {2015-11-06},
journal = {Journal of sleep research},
author = {Bastuji, Hélène and Perrin, Fabien and Garcia-Larrea, Luis},
year = {2003},
keywords = {Adult, Coercion, Disorders of Excessive Somnolence, Electroencephalography, Event-Related Potentials, P300, Evoked Potentials, Auditory, Female, Humans, Male, Narcolepsy, Sleep, REM, Wakefulness},
pages = {189--206},
file = {Bastuji et al_2003_Event-related potentials during forced awakening.pdf:C\:\\Users\\Raphael\\AppData\\Roaming\\Mozilla\\Firefox\\Profiles\\mm1r2zj7.default-1442859820820\\zotero\\storage\\WRHBPR76\\Bastuji et al_2003_Event-related potentials during forced awakening.pdf:application/pdf;Bastuji et al_2003_Event-related potentials during forced awakening.pdf:C\:\\Users\\Raphael\\AppData\\Roaming\\Mozilla\\Firefox\\Profiles\\mm1r2zj7.default-1442859820820\\zotero\\storage\\J5XHIQZ2\\Bastuji et al_2003_Event-related potentials during forced awakening.pdf:application/pdf}
}
@article{peter-derex_heterogeneity_2015,
title = {Heterogeneity of arousals in human sleep: {A} stereo-electroencephalographic study},
volume = {123},
issn = {10538119},
shorttitle = {Heterogeneity of arousals in human sleep},
url = {http://linkinghub.elsevier.com/retrieve/pii/S1053811915006771},
doi = {10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.07.057},
language = {en},
urldate = {2015-12-04},
journal = {NeuroImage},
author = {Peter-Derex, Laure and Magnin, Michel and Bastuji, Hélène},
month = dec,
year = {2015},
pages = {229--244},
file = {Peter-Derex et al_2015_Heterogeneity of arousals in human sleep.pdf:C\:\\Users\\Raphael\\AppData\\Roaming\\Mozilla\\Firefox\\Profiles\\mm1r2zj7.default-1442859820820\\zotero\\storage\\2QC3MJSS\\Peter-Derex et al_2015_Heterogeneity of arousals in human sleep.pdf:application/pdf}
}
@article{koulack_dream_1976,
title = {Dream recall and dream recall failure: {An} arousal-retrieval model.},
volume = {83},
number = {5},
journal = {Psychological Bulletin},
author = {Koulack, David and Goodenough, Donald R.},
year = {1976},
pages = {975--984},
file = {Koulack et Goodenough - 1976 - Dream recall and dream recall failure An arousal-.pdf:C\:\\Users\\Raphael\\AppData\\Roaming\\Mozilla\\Firefox\\Profiles\\mm1r2zj7.default-1442859820820\\zotero\\storage\\JQHQ7VUE\\Koulack et Goodenough - 1976 - Dream recall and dream recall failure An arousal-.pdf:application/pdf}
}
@article{koukkou_dreaming:_1983,
title = {Dreaming: {The} functional state-shift hypothesis. {A} neuropsychophysiological model},
volume = {142},
issn = {0007-1250},
shorttitle = {Dreaming},
url = {http://bjp.rcpsych.org/cgi/doi/10.1192/bjp.142.3.221},
doi = {10.1192/bjp.142.3.221},
language = {en},
number = {3},
urldate = {2016-03-24},
journal = {The British Journal of Psychiatry},
author = {Koukkou, M. and Lehmann, D.},
month = mar,
year = {1983},
pages = {221--231},
file = {Koukkou_Lehmann_1983_Dreaming.PDF:C\:\\Users\\Raphael\\AppData\\Roaming\\Mozilla\\Firefox\\Profiles\\mm1r2zj7.default-1442859820820\\zotero\\storage\\EDG6K7HS\\Koukkou_Lehmann_1983_Dreaming.PDF:application/pdf}
}
@article{campbell_perception_1981,